Добавить в цитаты Настройки чтения

Страница 36 из 55



“Go!” I exclaimed.

Kat released her and Callie darted to the plate. Without any hesitation, she licked up the hot dog first and then the pea. So far, so good.

“Hot dog!” I called out.

Feeling very pleased with my ingenuity, we set up to try again, except this time I reversed the location of the hot dog and pea. I nodded to Kat, and she released Callie. Once again, she made a beeline to the plate.

And lapped up the pea.

Okay, I thought, we can’t expect perfection. Maybe she was just excited.

“She ate the pea,” I called out. “Let’s try again.” Kat rolled her eyes, but humored me anyway. We repeated this ten times, and every single time Callie went to the left side of the plate, which is where the hot dog was located on the first trial.

“Maybe she knows that she will get both treats, and that’s why she goes to the same side,” Kat said.

Yes, of course. Must think like a dog. If I were Callie, I would just scoop up whatever was closest and move on to the next one since I would be getting both anyway.

“What if I picked up the treat she doesn’t choose first?” I said. “That way, she will have to make a choice.”

We reloaded the plate, and Kat let her go. Just like before, Callie lapped up the pea on the left side of the plate. This time, I snatched the hot dog away just as she started to make a move toward it.

If a dog could shrug its shoulders, Callie surely would have. She trotted back to Kat and waited for another round. But in the end it didn’t seem to make any difference. Callie just kept going to the left side, where the pea was placed. How could she prefer a pea to a hot dog? The hot dog was loaded with carnivorous goodness, perfectly suited to her Paleolithic instincts.

After about ten trials with the pea on the left, she finally paused and noticed the hot dog on the right side. In fact, this was the first time I had ever seen her pause with food in front of her nose. As if to say, Hey, where did this come from? she went for it.

And then she was stuck on the right.

Hot dog or pea, it didn’t make a difference. No matter how many times I placed a pea on the right, she wouldn’t track the hot dog.

Kat shook her head and said, “Do you still need me?”

“No,” I said. “I have another idea.”

I looked at Lyra, who had been watching the experiment from the sidelines. She had long gotten used to seeing Callie work for treats. If she waited long enough, she too would get to partake in the spoils, just for looking pretty. She perked up when I turned to her.

“Lyra, come here, sweetie!”

The three of us—Callie, Lyra, and I—padded into the kitchen.

Now, with the plate on the counter, I placed a pea on the left side of the plate and a hot dog on the right. Both dogs were rapt with anticipation. Quickly, I placed the plate on the floor.

As expected, Callie lurched for the right side, where she was still fixated. Before she could nab the hot dog, I grabbed it away and fed it to Lyra. A momentary look of confusion flashed across Callie’s face. Lyra was delighted and started to drool.



Surely, I thought, this would make Callie think about her choice.

It didn’t. Even now, with Callie’s usual avarice, she continued to perseverate on one side of the plate. Either she really didn’t care about the difference between hot dogs and peas, or her brain was executing a simple rule: stick with the same side as long as it has something good.

The next day, I asked Mark about Callie’s tendency to stick to one side.

“That’s common,” he said. “Some dogs are naturally right- or left-sided. Other dogs will remain with whatever attracted them first. Some will remain with wherever they were rewarded last. Other dogs will relax and cognitively evaluate each situation or wait for a cue.”

In fact, the development of a side preference was documented in a series of cognitive experiments in dogs in 2007. Researchers at the University of Michigan were attempting to determine if dogs had a concept of quantity. Does a dog know that two pieces of food are better than one? It seems obvious to us humans, but if you think about it, “quantity” is really quite an advanced concept. It requires some knowledge of the physics of the world, that larger volumes hold more stuff, and that more is better. Although there is some evidence that infants can discriminate basic differences between, say, one and two objects, the cognitive skill called numeracy doesn’t fully develop in humans until early childhood.

The researchers wanted to know if dogs had abilities similar to human infants. They tested twenty-nine dogs on a task very similar to what I had concocted in my kitchen. Plates with different amounts of food were offered to the dogs, and it was observed which plate the dogs chose. Most of the dogs chose the plate with more food, although not all the time. It was not clear whether the dogs actually had a sense of quantity or whether they were responding to perceptual cues of bigger piles of food. Either way, it was also noted that eight of the dogs had to be excluded from the analysis because they developed a side preference regardless of the quantity presented.

So Callie had a strong tendency for side preference, which seems to be a normal variant among dogs. Still, I was disappointed that my feisty rescue wasn’t an Einstein. I had no idea about McKenzie’s preferences, but since at least one of our subjects couldn’t tell us the difference between peas and hot dogs, a change in the experiment was in order.

The next day, I reported my findings to the lab.

Andrew was disappointed. “If they don’t care about the difference between peas and hot dogs, how will our experiment work?”

“It won’t,” I replied. “If peas and hot dogs are the same to the dogs, then the hand signals convey no useful information. As long as they put their head on the rest, they know they will get a treat. They don’t care which.”

Nobody could understand why the dogs didn’t discriminate between the two foods. We were all stuck thinking like humans. We had to think like a dog.

“What if we just get rid of the peas?” I mused.

“You mean a reward versus no-reward experiment?” Andrew asked.

“Exactly. Even if the dogs don’t care about hot dogs versus peas, surely they care about hot dogs versus nothing.”

Andrew nodded in agreement.

It wouldn’t even require any new training. We already had the two hand signals. Left hand up meant “hot dog.” Now, two hands pointing toward each other would mean “no hot dog” instead of “pea.”

“Don’t you think the dogs will get irritated and stop doing the task?” Andrew asked.

It was a good question. If it were me in the MRI simulator, I would scoot right out of there as soon as I realized I wasn’t getting food all the time. Psychologists call this extinction, which means that if you stop rewarding a previously learned behavior, the behavior will eventually stop.

But dogs might see it differently. Not rewarding every trial might increase their motivation. This is called variable reinforcement—VR for short. VR is very common in animal experiments. A VR10 schedule means that sometimes the subject is rewarded but, on average, only once every ten trials. The unpredictability of VR tends to make animals more attentive and work harder to obtain the reward.

Something as drastic as VR10 would not work in our experiment. I just couldn’t see Callie sitting still for ten repetitions of hand signals to get just a tiny cube of hot dog. If I were in her position, I would begin to wonder after the third repetition without a treat. By about the fifth repetition without food, I would probably give up entirely and quit the experiment. I suspected Callie wouldn’t put up with it either. More important, it would result in an imbalance in the number of observations collected in the sca